bio342_poster_session_Dec6
Crickets and Flies
Food Fight!  The Effects of Octopamine on Resource Competition in Crickets
    J. Leonard and B. Susko                                                      poster.pdf                      
Chump to Champ: Increasing Cricket Aggression
    Christine Beckel and Christine Deyo    
Kids of today: Female Mate Choice in Juvenile House Crickets (Acheta domesticus).
    Emily Kam, and Seyram Ahiadeke Butame                        poster.pdf
Individual Foraging Behavior in Drosophila melanogaster
    Angela Gibbs and Amy Campbell                                       poster.pdf 
Fish (non-electric)
Brave Guppies: A Study in Prey Response
    Kate Williams                                          
Female Guppy Preference for her Ancestral Lineage: an Example of Divergence through Sexual Selection
    Molly Schumer and Jennifer Jin                                            poster.pdf
Follow the Light; Light Cueing in Cichlids
    William C. McNitt    
Participation in Scientific Research Causes Mothers and Young to Become Unfit for Survival Outside of Academic Institutions
    Douglas T. Borst     
Fighting Females: Effect of Size on Female-Female Aggression in A. burtoni
    Julia Carlton and Johnathon Packer                                        poster.pdf
The Residual Effects of Atrazine Exposure in Goldfish.
    Tiffany Cook    
Fish (electric)
An Investigation of the Correlation Between Field Lateralization and Prostrate Side Preference in the S. American Benthopelagic Electric Knifefish Rhamphichthys rostratus.
    Noah  Oppenheim and Garon Coriz   
Apteronotus albifrons Play Hide-and-Seek All Day Long
    Aroon Karra, Amanda Trail, Alex Winters, B.
Fish Foraging: Disturbing the Foraging Behavior of Gnathonemus petersii:
    Laila Bryant     
Field (outside the lab) Biology
Diving Mallards
    Evan Bremer    
Analyzing Behavior in Captive Cougars
    R. Bond and P. Guzman        
Take to the Trees: Patterns in Sciurus carolinensis Behavior.
    K.C. Regmi, E.A. Meloche-Justusson                                    poster.pdf
Social Behaviors of Captive Fruit Bat Species
    Jacob Schwartzman, Jamie McNellis                                       poster.pdf
Now there’s no excuse for bad behavior: Dogs do not find training more stressful than play.
    A Mahan        
Amphibians
The Lonely Tadpole: A Study of Kin Recognition in Xenopus laevis.
    Natalie C. Vizcarra and C. A. Mingoya                                   poster.pdf
Fire-Bellied Toads are Ambidextrous
    Juliana M. Arrighi and Mikella Procopio    
Honky-Tonk: The Role of Communication in Canada Geese
Lacy Lackey and Georgia Kirkpatrick      
 
ABSTRACTS

Food Fight!  The Effects of Octopamine on Resource Competition in Crickets
J. Leonard and B. Susko

Researchers have observed aggressive competition for resources in isolated and starved crickets.  In these competitions, the initial possessor of the food pellet usually wins [1].  Octopamine, a neurotransmitter similar to adrenaline, increases aggressive tendencies in crickets [2].  We tested whether octopamine injections increased aggressive behavior during resource competition and whether a non-possessor would be able to out-compete a possessor under these conditions.  Crickets were starved and anesthetized with CO2 then injected with either 10% ringer solution (blank) or octopamine (10-2 M).  Possessors with octopamine or ringer solution injections were tested against non-possessors with octopamine or ringer solution injections.  Of the eleven trials performed, only four had a determinable winner.  These results support the results of other literature by showing that possessors are more likely to remain in possession of the food than non-possessors.  We found no correlation between success in food competition and octopamine injections.  
1. Nosil P (2002). Food Fights in House Crickets, Achetas domesticus, and the Effects of Body Size and Hunger Level. Canadian Journal of Zoology.
2. Matsumoto Y, Sakai M (2001). Brain Control of Mating Behavior in the Male Cricket Gryllus bimaculatus DeGeer: Excitatory Control of Copulatory Actions. Zoological Science 18:659-669. poster.pdf
top


Chump to champ: Increasing Cricket Aggression
Christine Beckel and Christine Deyo

It has been concluded that cricket aggression is regulated by the hormone octopamine. It has also been found that the act of flying increases octopamine levels, or resets them to normal levels once they have been depressed, usually resulting from losing a fight. We looked at the effects that flying, swimming, running and immobilization have on aggression levels in the house cricket Achata domesticus. We used an ethogram to measure the aggressive behaviors during fights between males. The losers were then subjected to one of the treatments, and fought again against a new cricket. We found that the only treatment with a significant (p = .024) effect on aggression levels was the flying, although swimming and running may have had a slight increase, while immobilization may have had a slight decrease in aggression.
top


Kids of today: Female mate choice in juvenile house crickets (Acheta domesticus).
Emily Kam, and Seyram Ahiadeke Butame

Current literature shows that for different species of crickets there is a general preference for large males by female crickets as mates. It has been shown in many cases that the male size corresponds to the quality of his song, which females use as a measure of superior fitness. We tried to determine if this mate choice behavior could be detected in juvenile crickets, 3-week and 4-week olds. Female crickets were placed in a tank in between two males of different sizes. In treatment one, the females were able to see the male crickets, but the male crickets could not see the females. Treatment two, the females and the males could see each other. In the control males of similar size were used and males and females could see each other. Female position relative to each male was recorded every minute for ten minutes for each trial, with five trials per treatment. Our results suggested that female crickets have a preference for larger males. However, statistical analysis of our data showed that in many cases this might have been due to chance and not conscious selection on the part of the female. poster.pdf
top



Individual foraging behavior in Drosophila melanogaster
Amy Campbell and Angela Gibbs

The foraging gene (for) in Drosophila melanogaster provides a rare example of a single gene that underlies a naturally occurring complex behavior. Two variants of this gene have been isolated; individuals with the rover allele (forR) move greater distances while feeding than do individuals that are homozygous for the sitter allele (forS). This difference is present in both larval and adult life stages. However, whether individual variation in food-search strategy is consistent from the larval to adult stage for a given individual has never been investigated. In order to address this question, the foraging behavior of individuals from populations of forR and forS flies was examined at both larval and adult developmental stages and then individuals were genotyped to confirm the allele. While unable to support the hypothesis that individual foraging behavior is consistent between life stages, some preliminary data suggests that this is the case. Modifications to the adult foraging behavior protocol and repetition of the experiment would provide more conclusive results. poster.pdf 
top


Brave Guppies: A Study in Prey Response
Kate Williams

A prey’s response to its predator can be a life-changing behavior. Within a species, there should be variation in response to a predator based on phenotype because some phenotypes are more easily detected. Guppies provide a good model for this phenotype study because there are two easily identified groups of phenotype: small, dull wild type guppies and large, bright “fancy” guppies. They also have distinct predator-response behaviors. In this study, two behaviors were modeled – surface skim evasion and inspection. To look at these behaviors, horizontal and vertical movement towards and away from the predator in the test tank was observed and indices for these movements were calculated and used as a basis of comparison. The results, however, showed little difference both in predator response between phenotypes and in the presence or absence of the predator in general. The only significant difference in behavior occurred in horizontal movement, with the larger, brighter fish swimming farther from the predator and the wild type swimming closer to the predator. However, previous studies have shown that there should potentially be little response to predators in lab guppies. To draw true conclusions from this sort of data, therefore, one would need to repeat this experiment using guppies collected from a natural environment.  
top


Female Guppy preference for her ancestral lineage: an example of divergence through sexual selection
Molly Schumer and Jennifer Jin

Previous research has shown partial reproductive isolation between two lineages of Poecilia reticulata, the common guppy and the endlers livebearer. Although there is no evidence of genetic incompatibility between these population, they diverge both morphologically and in mating display behavior, and rarely interbreed (Alexander and Breden, 2004). Female guppy preference for males of her ancestral lineage may be adaptive; native males are likely to have traits better suited for survival in that environment than foreign males, and females may select genes that are successful in their environment when they choose a native male. Such sexual selection creates prezygotic isolation that has already produced morphological and genetic divergence (Alexander and Breeden, 2004), and may eventually result in a speciation event. Our study investigated whether female guppy mate choice drives reproductive isolation between the two populations, and whether females are primarily using visual or behavioral mechanisms to differentiate between males. In both choice tests, females preferred the male guppies, demonstrating that female mate choice could be the cause of low levels of interbreeding. In the first experiment, females evaluated males based on visual stimuli. Though research has shown that females favor orange coloration (Dugatkin and Godin, 1998), females preferred male guppies, even though male endlers were significantly more orange. In the second experiment, females chose males based on both visual and behavioral stimuli; male endlers did not display, and females again preferred guppies. However, there was no significant difference between female preference for guppies in the first and second experiments, suggesting that females are primarily using visual cues to differentiate between males. poster.pdf
top


Follow the Light; Light Cueing in Cichlids
William C. McNitt

Species of cichlid fish in Lake Victoria demonstrate mate preference based on red and blue color morphs, and furthermore there appears to be a correlation between color morph, range of vision, and environmental ambient light conditions (Maan et al., 2006).  The purpose of this research is to investigate the ability of cichlid fish to detect various colors of light in the vertical plane and to determine if the cichlids have a preference for a particular color.  In order to investigate this, a small population of cichlids was first exposed to a regimen that combined light stimuli with the appearance of food and then tested for the ability to later react to light stimuli as though food were going to appear.  No significant differences between groups were detected, however this is most likely due to a flaw in the experimental design.  Further research should be conducted with modified experimental conditions in order to obtain meaningful data.
top


Participation in scientific research causes mothers and young to become unfit for survival outside of academic institutions
Douglas T. Borst

Astatotilapa burtoni is a species of cichlid fish that is commonly used in genetic research.  Despite their great use in this fields, little is known about their patterns of behavior in reproduction and care of young.  In this study I explored several experimental methods that could be used to quantify the effects of motherhood and genetics on the behavior of cichlid fry.  While few trials were run because of the constraints of time and the availability of experimental subjects, initial results do suggest that stocks of A. burtoni raised in captivity for extended periods have picked up behavioral traits that would decrease fitness. Wild cichlid fry are much more likely to seek out their mother and swim near the bottom of the water column when placed in a new environment, while lab-stock seem to display much more ambivalence towards both their mother and their position in the water column.  The experiments run were not sufficient to determine weather this effect was the result of behavioral changes in the mother, or genetic changes in the fry.
top


Fighting Females: Effect of Size on Female-Female Aggression in A. burtoni
Julia Carlton and Jonathan Packer

Female aggressive behavior is common in cichlids, but not much is known about the circumstances under which it occurs. We looked at one potential factor, body length, in the African cichlid Astatotilapia burtoni, to better understand the nature of female aggression. We isolated females of varying life history stages (all wild A. burtoni) and then placed them in a divided resource-free tank with one female on each side, allowing 10 minutes for habituation. We removed the divider, and we filmed 5-minute bouts of female-female interactions and male-female interactions, with 16 fish of varying body length, in this resource-free environment over the course of 2 weeks. We analyzed their behavior using focal observation with a 5-point ethogram and a model for aggression both based on a Texas cichlid study on the effect of size (length) on male aggression. This study found a significant effect for male-male interaction, but not male-female. Much like the Texas cichlid study, we found that length does not have a significant effect on aggressive behavior in male-female interactions, nor in female-female interactions in A. burtoni. This suggests that there must be some other factor at work, as female aggressive behavior clearly varies. It is likely that the female ovarian cycle is a better indicator of aggressive behavior, a factor that must be studied further. poster.pdf
top


The Residual Effects of Atrazine Exposure in Goldfish
Tiffany Cook

One of the most common pesticides present as a pollutant in the Willamette River, according to the USGS, is Atrazine.  These experiments focus on the long term effects that a short exposure to this herbicide has on Carassius auratus, or the common feeder goldfish.  

Goldfish were exposed overnight to three different concentrations of Atrazine (0.5, 5, and 50 µg/L), then transported back to a tank of pure water where they remained for the remainder of the first experiment.  Burst swimming was used as a measurement of the level of stress upon the goldfish.  By counting “burst swims,” or sudden darts with a clear stop, it was found that, while overnight exposures to Atrazine did alter the fish behavior, after several days, the behavior of the fish returned to its original state (as compared to a control group of fish).  It was also found that between two and four days after exposure, the goldfish exhibited maximum burst swimming behaviors.

When this experiment was completed, a second was conducted to determine if the effects of Atrazine are additive.  The same goldfish were exposed again to the same doses of Atrazine for the exact same amount of time.  Burst swimming was again used as the form of stress level measurement.  When compared to the data from the first experiment, no significant difference was found.  Therefore, the effects of Atrazine on burst swimming behavior are not additive.  
top


An investigation of the correlation between field lateralization and prostrate side preference in the South American benthopelagic electric knifefish Rhamphichthys rostratus.
Noah  Oppenheim and Garon Coriz

A correlation was observed between field lateralization and prostrate side preference in the pulse-gymnotid Rhamphichthys rostratus. This study attempted to statistically demonstrate that correlation using five individuals over the course of three weeks. Rhamphichthys rostratus is a benthopelagic gymnotiform native to the costal rivers of northeastern South America (reference http://filaman.ifm-geomar.de/Summary/SpeciesSummary.php?id=28040). Side preference was measured using scan sampling of individual fish positions two to three times per day. Electric field lateralization was determined by measuring the voltage emitted by the fish at specific points adjacent to the fish as far as five cm away. Correlations were drawn from scores of right/left side position preference and scores of relatively stronger field from relative points on the right and left sides. One fish out of the five exhibited a strong correlation between field lateralization and side preference (p = 0.031 left and 0.0078 left for ethogram and field mapping sides, respectively). All other fish exhibited significant field lateralization (p < 0.001). One other exhibited significant slight side preference (p = 0.0192), the side opposite its field lateralization.
top


Apteronotus albifrons Play Hide-and-Seek All Day Long
Aroon Karra, Amanda Trail, Alex Winters

Apteronotus albifrons is a weakly electric fish from South America that generates a discharge from an electric organ that is believed to be used for navigation, object detection and communication. These fish are also nocturnal and hide in select retreat sites during the day. From previous studies concerning a closely related fish, Apteronotus leptorhynchus, it was shown that there were clear retreat site preferences amongst fish; dominance relationships determined by the fish’s frequency were also shown to exist.  Our study assessed whether these same relationships existed in A. albifrons.  We first assessed retreat site preferences in A. albifrons by providing individual fish with tubes that differed in color, length and diameter. In individual tests, fish had a strong preference for black tubes over white tubes. There was no significant preference in tubes of differing diameter.  Also, there was no significant preference in length due to non-independence of tube preference and tank side preference.  We then assessed group preference by grouping five fish into a single tank with six tubes that differed in color, length and diameter.  Group testing showed that fish preferred black tubes over white tubes and tubes with a length of six and eight inches over a length of four inches.  There was also an association between high electric organ discharge frequencies in grouped fish and preferred tube selection in variable color and length tubes.


Fish Foraging: Disturbing the Foraging Behavior of Gnathonemus petersii:
Laila Bryant

The weakly electric fish Gnathonemus petersii uses a pulse-type EOD to electrolocate and search for food at night. By establishing a feeding routine, I was able to introduce different perturbations of the environment of the fish and evaluate whether they affected the foraging behavior of G. petersii. This study was focused on how these fish use their EOD as a means of locating food, and how electrical noise designed to block the fish’s own electric pulse affects behavior and foraging success. The fish’s environment was disturbed by many things, including the experimenter, the carbon rods which were used to input the noise, as well as the electric noise itself. The main element which would improve the fishes ability to deal with the stress of these things is time to acclimate to them, which, unfortunately, was not available in this study.
top


Diving Mallards
Evan Bremer

Several species of ducks dive to obtain food or escape predators, the local species being bufflehead ducks, lesser scaups, and ruddy ducks.  They are physiologically adapted to the behavior, being able to express a significant amount of air from their bodies and feathers immediately before diving and recover it upon surfacing.  Their body shape is also streamlined for optimal movement underwater.  It is surprising, then, that some mallards, a species that lacks the ability to change the amount of air they carry, and have a body design optimized for movement at the water's surface, have learned to dive.  From observations, one can conclude that they are not very good at it.  So how did they learn? I assumed that the circumstances under which they dive most are the circumstances under which they learned, and observed them with attention to their diving when they were given food by humans and when given no handouts.  Data collection at this time is not complete and conditions are not ideal, but thus far it looks as if they learned to dive after food thrown to them by humans.
top


Analyzing Behavior in Captive Cougars
R. Bond and P. Guzman

A vast amount of research has been conducted on the hunting behaviors of wild cougars, while little to no research has been conducted on captive cougars.  The purpose of this project was to determine the behaviors of two captive cougars at the Portland Zoo.  In order to observe cougars an ethogram was developed containing eight behaviors.  This would allow the cougars’ behavior to be quantified and more easily analyzed.  The cougars were observed for seven and a half hours over three separate days.  The experiment resulted in the knowledge that captive cougars spend the majority of the day resting, observing, and walking.  By comparing each cougar’s overall behavior pattern it was concluded that there was a substantial difference in behavior between the two.  By contrasting each day of observation the combined cougars behavior appear to vary by day.  A comparison of the two observers shows that the data was not seriously affected by observer variation.  

The information that this study provided has many uses for future research.  With field research a comparison of wild and captive behaviors can be made.  The research we have completed can also be applied to analyze the effectiveness of the animal enrichment program at the zoo.
top


Take to the Trees: Patterns in Sciurus carolinensis Behavior.
K.C. Regmi, E.A. Meloche-Justusson

Although squirrels are known to have bimodal daily activity patterns, the number of squirrels present at various times on Reed College campus did not appear to simply depend on time of day based on informal observation.  For this reason, it was hypothesized that weather or temperature may also play a role in squirrel activity. Squirrels were observed by scan sampling in two locations over a three-week period. Temperature, weather, and time of day were noted for each sample. Behaviors were also recorded to determine whether the types of behavior are correlated with any of these variables. Results varied between the two locations. The number of squirrels seen in a day was found to have only time-dependence in one location. In the other location, nothing had a significant effect on squirrel number, although this difference could be due to a non-linear distribution; in the group with no significant time-of-day effect, it appeared that there were fewer squirrels in both early morning and late afternoon. The number of squirrels was not significantly correlated with weather or temperature. In both locations, the proportion of time spent eating significantly increased as it got later in the season, even accounting for temperature differences. One possible explanation is that changing nut availability has an effect on behavior. Future experiments could explore the effect of nut supply by seeing if the density of nuts remaining on the ground has a significant effect on behavior type. poster.pdf
top


Social Behaviors of Captive Fruit Bat Species
Jacob Schwartzman, Jamie McNellis

We developed an ethogram to describe and examine the social interactions of three species of fruit bats at the Oregon Zoo.  The enriched zoo exhibit includes a majority breeding population of Straw-colored fruit bats (Eidolon helvum), eight male Egyptian fruit bats (Rousettus aegyptiacus), and a nonbreeding population of Rodrigues flying foxes (Pteropus rodricensis).  While the Egyptian and Straw-colored bats are morphologically similar and have overlapping habitats (sub-Saharan Africa), the Rodrigues bats are an endangered species native to Rodrigues Island near Madagascar, and were imported as part of a conservation program.  We therefore hypothesized that there should be detectable differences in interspecies behavior in the exhibit.  We observed that most interactions were either inspection (sniffing) or aggressive behaviors such as “wing boxing” or grappling.  Though the exhibit provides a convenient arena in which to track individual bats, our analysis was hampered by the rapidity of interactions and the difficulty of determining species and/or sex of the target of an individual’s behavior.  Despite this, we have identified significant differences in the behavioral patterns of species and sex.  poster.pdf
top


Now there’s no excuse for bad behavior: Dogs do not find training more stressful than play.
A Mahan

Domesticated dogs have long been known to derive pleasure from human contact.  A decrease in stress, as measured by cortisol concentrations, has been shown to occur when humans interact with dogs during play.  This work sought to characterize how stress induction changes when dogs interact with humans in a more structured way, as during training.  Eighteen dogs were used in this study, with thirty-seven samples collected.  Approximately half of the test dogs were exposed to twenty minutes of play, while the other half had twenty minutes of training.  Saliva samples were taken before and after the human interaction and these were used to determine cortisol concentrations by ELISA.

It was hypothesized that there was no significant difference in stress induction when dogs were exposed to either type of human contact.  Analysis of the results shows that there is no significant difference between the change in stress levels during play or training.  This surprising result suggests that despite owners’ suspicions, dogs, on average, do not find training to be stressful.  This may be due to artificial selection pressures that humans create when breeding dogs for obedience and temperament.

As an additional parameter for investigation, half of the dogs tested were service animals in training with Guide Dogs for the Blind, Inc.  These animals showed no significant difference in stress induction, though they varied more on average in their stress levels.
top


Fire-bellied toads are ambidextrous
Juliana M. Arrighi and Mikella Procopio

Laterality, or handedness, has been shown in several frog species in both tadpoles and  etamorphosed individuals and is probably related to asymmetries in development.  Most species displaying this behavior have a single lateral spiracle.  To our knowledge, only one other study has examined Bombina orientalis, which have double spiracles, for laterality.  We obtained offspring from wild caught B. orientalis and raised them at a constant temperature of 23 C.  Recently metamorphosed Bombina orientalis frogs were presented with a threat stimulus from either the front or back by one of two experimenters.  The frogs' locomotory responses were scored for direction (right, left or none), angle, and distance.  While we found no evidence of laterality in B. orientalis, stimulus direction and experimenter had an effect on whether or not the frog turned.  When stimulated from behind, frogs tended not to exhibit a directional response, while those presented with stimulus from the front almost always exhibited directional response.
top


THE LONELY TADPOLE: A STUDY OF KIN RECOGNITION IN Xenopus laevis
Natalie C. Vizcarra and C. A. Mingoya

Kin recognition systems in developing animals serve a wide range of protective purposes. Recognition and differentiation of ones own species, offspring and birth clutch serve an important role in facilitating appropriate parental care and successfully directing attempts at mating and competitive or protective aggression.  The existence of larval kin recognition has been shown in many anuran species; however, specific data sets have yet to be released for Xenopus laevis. To test aggregation preferences in two unrelated sibling groups of Xenopus laevis tadpoles, a simple Y-tube arena made of transparent plastics was used to test the hypothesis that when frightened, tadpoles prefer to aggregate with their kin. Four trials with N=20 were done to establish a control for tadpole preference between a sibling laden or empty tank. Two subsequent experiments were performed to test for tadpole density preference and preference for sibling over non-sibling. All results were statistically shown to be insignificant as the tadpoles failed to show any preference. Further analysis of the data to determine a potential tank side preference was also insignificant. Further experimentation must occur in order to make significant conclusions about preferential aggregation in Xenopus laevis tadpoles.   poster.pdf
top


Honky-tonk: The Role of Communication in Canada Geese
Lacy Lackey and Georgia Kirkpatrick

In our project, we were interested in how and why Canada Geese use calls, or "honks".  We observed geese in the Crystal Springs Rhododendron Garden to determine the associations between behavior, group dynamics, and vocalizations.   In addition to scan samplings, we also took audio recordings with a Marantz portable cassette recorder PMD222 (and a big fancy microphone), and later analyzed the recordings to assess the frequency and characteristics of calls.  Correlating this information revealed that Canada Geese use calls in situations such as group movement, aggression, reunion of group members, and flight.  Calls also had specific characteristics in each situation.  These distinguishing characteristics included relative change in frequency of calls with behavior.  In literature that we read chracterizing other forms of goose calls, this measurement was not used.
top